Peter Santema's Avatar

Peter Santema

@petersantema.bsky.social

Ornithology / Behavioural Ecology / Natural History. Researcher at dept. of Ornithology at Max Planck Institute for Biological Intelligence.

191 Followers  |  183 Following  |  28 Posts  |  Joined: 05.04.2024  |  2.3294

Latest posts by petersantema.bsky.social on Bluesky

These findings show that redstart chicks can learn to recognize novel alarm calls by associating them with familiar alarm calls. This demonstrates that nestling birds eavesdrop on the alarm calls of other species through acoustic association.

16.07.2025 05:58 β€” πŸ‘ 1    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 0    πŸ“Œ 0
Video thumbnail

When we first played back the novel calls, the nestlings showed no response. We then played the unfamiliar call six times together with redstart alarm calls. After this training phase, nestlings responded to the novel call in the same way as to alarm calls of their own species.

16.07.2025 05:58 β€” πŸ‘ 2    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0

However, parents are not always around to warn them. Alarm calls from other species may then provide an additional valuable source of information. We tested whether nestling redstarts can learn to recognize unfamiliar calls by associating them with known alarm calls from their own species.

16.07.2025 05:58 β€” πŸ‘ 1    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0

Predators pose a major threat to nestling birds, so it is important that nestlings recognize and respond to signals that indicate danger. When their parents make alarm calls, for instance, chicks typically stop begging and crouch down to avoid being detected.

16.07.2025 05:58 β€” πŸ‘ 1    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Post image

New paper led by Jinggang Zhang just out in @royalsocietypublishing.org! We show that nestling redstarts learn to respond to calls of other species if hear them together with alarm calls of their own species doi.org/10.1098/rspb...

16.07.2025 05:58 β€” πŸ‘ 12    πŸ” 4    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0

However, parents are not always around to warn them. Alarm calls from other species may then provide an additional valuable source of information. We tested whether nestling redstarts can learn to recognize unfamiliar calls by associating them with known alarm calls from their own species.

16.07.2025 05:51 β€” πŸ‘ 0    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 0    πŸ“Œ 0

Predators pose a major threat to nestling birds, so it is important that nestlings recognize and respond to signals that indicate danger. When their parents make alarm calls, for instance, chicks typically stop begging and crouch down to avoid being detected.

16.07.2025 05:51 β€” πŸ‘ 0    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0

Spectacular!

08.06.2025 07:18 β€” πŸ‘ 1    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Preview
To Bees or Not to Bees: Greater Honeyguides Sometimes Guide Humans to Animals Other Than Bees, but Likely Not as Punishment We show that greater honeyguides guide humans to nonbee destinations (snakes and a dead mammal); yet this is a rare occurrence, happening in only 3.7% of human-honeyguide interactions in 1 year and 0...

Honeyguides lead you to 🍯, but if you don't reward the bird, next time it will guide you to a lion 🦁! So goes the story, but do honeyguides actually guide to dangerous animals, and is it punishment? New research suggests they do, but it's more "oops" than "revenge!" doi.org/10.1002/ece3...

30.04.2025 10:36 β€” πŸ‘ 38    πŸ” 17    πŸ’¬ 0    πŸ“Œ 1

We conclude that breeding site fidelity may be a beneficial strategy for individuals that are successful in a certain location, even in species that are generally nomadic. Big thanks to the co-authors and the many people that contributed to data collection! 6/6

24.03.2025 08:49 β€” πŸ‘ 2    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 0    πŸ“Œ 0
Post image

Males that returned to the study area also had higher siring success and stayed longer in the study area than males that had not been in the study area the previous year 5/6

24.03.2025 08:49 β€” πŸ‘ 1    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Post image

Males that had higher siring success and had stayed in the study area for longer were more likely to return 4/6

24.03.2025 08:49 β€” πŸ‘ 1    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Post image

However, a very small proportion of ~900 colour banded pectoral sandpipers did return to their former breeding site in the subsequent breeding season, with males returning more often (2.4%) than females (0.5%) 3/6

24.03.2025 08:49 β€” πŸ‘ 1    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Post image

Satellite tagging showed that pectoral sandpipers typically do not return anywhere near their former breeding site, with a median dispersal distance between years of >800 km for both males and females 2/6

24.03.2025 08:49 β€” πŸ‘ 2    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Post image

New paper on breeding site fidelity in pectoral sandpipers out in @asab.org! The pectoral sandpiper is a highly nomadic polygynous shorebird, but a very small proportion of individuals is nevertheless faithful to their breeding site between years 1/6 www.sciencedirect.com/science/arti...

24.03.2025 08:49 β€” πŸ‘ 33    πŸ” 10    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0

We conclude that breeding site fidelity may be a beneficial strategy for individuals that are successful in a certain location, even in species that are generally nomadic. Big thanks to the co-authors and the many people that contributed to data collection! 6/6

24.03.2025 08:42 β€” πŸ‘ 0    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 0    πŸ“Œ 0
Post image

Males that returned to the study area also had higher siring success and stayed longer in the study area than males that had not been in the study area the previous year 5/6

24.03.2025 08:42 β€” πŸ‘ 0    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Post image

Males that had higher siring success and had stayed in the study area for longer were more likely to return 4/6

24.03.2025 08:42 β€” πŸ‘ 0    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Post image

However, a very small proportion of ~900 colour banded pectoral sandpipers did return to their former breeding site in the subsequent breeding season, with males returning more often (2.4%) than females (0.5%) 3/6

24.03.2025 08:42 β€” πŸ‘ 0    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Post image

Satellite tagging showed that pectoral sandpipers typically do not return anywhere near their former breeding site, with a median dispersal distance between years of >800 km for both males and females 2/6

24.03.2025 08:42 β€” πŸ‘ 0    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Preview
The demographic drivers of cultural evolution in bird song Social learning can give rise to shared behavioral patterns that persist as culture within animal communities,1,2 such as bird and whale songs and cet…

New paper on the demographic drivers of cultural evolution in Great Tit song published today - epic work led by @nilomr.bsky.social with help from @andreaestandia.bsky.social Ella Cole & Sara Keen. Why did we do this, and what did we find? 🧡 follows: 1/n
www.sciencedirect.com/science/arti...

07.03.2025 17:30 β€” πŸ‘ 164    πŸ” 77    πŸ’¬ 8    πŸ“Œ 10
Preview
Male Ruff travel thousands of kilometres in lekking season A new study on Ruff movements has defied traditional ideas about bird migration and breeding, with some males covering up to 9,000 km in search of a mate during the nesting season.

How much effort have you put in this Valentine's Day?

Research has revealed that male Ruff travel thousands of kilometres in search of mating opportunities over the course of the breeding season:

14.02.2025 15:57 β€” πŸ‘ 15    πŸ” 5    πŸ’¬ 0    πŸ“Œ 1

Did you get it? Glad the book received a good home! πŸ˜…

13.02.2025 11:37 β€” πŸ‘ 0    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Preview
The Lost Birds of Paradise Buy The Lost Birds of Paradise First Edition by Fuller, Errol (ISBN: 9781853105661) from Amazon's Book Store. Everyday low prices and free delivery on eligible orders.

I am a sucker for natural history books, and occasionally find a really good offer for a book that I already have (see www.amazon.co.uk/dp/185310566X). Very tempted to buy this one as a 2nd copy, but that would open the floodgates. Someone please buy this fabulous book for less than a cup of coffee!

13.02.2025 11:24 β€” πŸ‘ 1    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0
Post image

Large-scale sampling of potential breeding sites in male ruffs | royalsocietypublishi... | Proceedings of the Royal Society B | #ornithology πŸͺΆ

09.01.2025 09:00 β€” πŸ‘ 14    πŸ” 1    πŸ’¬ 0    πŸ“Œ 0

We suggest that breeding site sampling on a large spatial scale may common in polygamous species with uniparental care, especially those that are migratory, have high reproductive skew, and show spatial variability in the timing of breeding 7/7

08.01.2025 07:07 β€” πŸ‘ 6    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 0    πŸ“Œ 0
Preview
Breeding site sampling across the Arctic by individual males of a polygynous shorebird - Nature Nomadic movement across the breeding range enables male pectoral sandpipers to display and sire offspring at multiple sites within a single breeding season, with tenure depending on breeding female nu...

Similar breeding site sampling on a large scale has previously been found in males of the polygynous pectoral sandpiper (see www.nature.com/articles/nat...), indicating that this behaviour is not unique to ruffs 6/7

08.01.2025 07:07 β€” πŸ‘ 8    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0

Ruffs are unique among birds in that there are three distinct mating strategies among males: aggressive β€œindependents”, submissive β€œsatellites”, and female mimicking β€œfaeders”. All three morphs displayed similar breeding site sampling behaviour 5/7

08.01.2025 07:07 β€” πŸ‘ 8    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0

Male ruffs thus show very high mobility during the breeding season, and search for mating opportunities across a large part of their breeding range 4/7

08.01.2025 07:07 β€” πŸ‘ 6    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0

We satellite-tagged 99 male ruffs shortly before the start of the breeding season. These males subsequently visited a median of 11 different sites, staying at each site for a median of two days, and travelling a median total distance of 4435 km 3/7

08.01.2025 07:07 β€” πŸ‘ 9    πŸ” 0    πŸ’¬ 1    πŸ“Œ 0

@petersantema is following 20 prominent accounts